Training Agility in Team-Based Sports; A Review
Background
Much of what we do within Strength and Conditioning (S&C) environments is focused on improving sport-specific actions involving agility. Competitive, Team-Based Sports (TBS) in particular, frequently see unique moments or phases of play which are unpredictable in nature – placing a decision-making demand upon the athlete (1). Consequently, the athlete must then execute the next skill upon perceiving the external cue and then the process repeats – this is commonly known as perception-action coupling. Therefore, in TBS, agility is a combination of two skills; the ability to perceive the environment correctly and the ability to change direction efficiently (1, 9).
Regarding training agility, a frequently used method is the Constraints-Led Approach (CLA) which looks at applying the Dynamic Systems Theory of motor learning (17). Typically, drills can be either blocked or random, utilizing varying levels of contextual interference. Recognizing which method is best demands good skill and awareness of the coach – considering the task complexity, athlete ability and technical demands of the sport.
This review will aim to assess the relevant literature around blocked and random programming and how it links with contextual interference – ultimately asking the question; can sport-specific training alone, provide an adequate agility stimulus? S&C practitioners are often given little time to work with athletes on such characteristics so this review will also aim to provide some practical takeaways for practitioners regarding how they might program agility within TBS and what level of contextual interference is considered optimal.
Team-Based Sports – A brief analysis
TBS typically involve attacking and defending periods of play, usually with a team defending one goal and trying to score in another. The games are random, intermittent and dynamic in nature, usually lasting between 50-90 minutes. Due to the chaotic nature of play, exact repeats in play are rarely seen. Where they can be predictable, they can often still be extremely varied. So you may see a 1v1 in one area of the pitch/court but 5 minutes later, see a 1v4 in the same area of the pitch. The mental, physical, technical and tactical components will all influence the perceptions an athlete faces which creates a very random and non-linear playing environment. Teams can range in size from 5-15 players and pitch/courts vary greatly from sport to sport (1).
Constraints-Led Approach
The CLA is a coaching approach, formed according to the theory that humans are dynamic, complex and self-organising systems. In skill acquisition environments, coaches commonly place constraints upon athletes to force them into certain positions or into desired habits (3). Newell’s model identifies 3 main constraints that can be manipulated in order to encourage certain behavior outcomes – the task, the environment and the player. This lends itself well to perception-action coupling approach which proposes that actions will occur based on cues or stimulus within the environment (17). An action will follow and thus the perception alters and the cycle repeats. Therefore, in order for agility to be specifically developed, training needs to replicate the actions that an athlete would encounter in a competitive environment. This allows them to make decisions and apply the movement skill in a realistic manner. However, considerations need to be made based on the athlete’s technical ability before getting too specific.
Practice Design: Random or Blocked and Contextual Interference
Random and Blocked practice design – both have benefits and drawbacks. Blocked practice is simple, repetitive and predictable often having a in a sole focus but can be limited in-terms of specificity to competitive-cues. Random, on the other hand is non-linear, may include multiple focuses and is more complex often being more like competition (5). Random practice poses more of a challenge for a performer both perceptually and technically (16). Across the two types of practice design, contextual interference moves from low, in blocked training to high, in random training. Contextual Interference can be defined as “the effect of learning of the degree of functional interference found in a practice situation when several tasks must be learned and practiced together” (7).
Wulf et al. (2010) found that tasks with low levels of contextual interference saw better acute skill performances whereas tasks with high contextual interference levels saw greater long-term skill retention – as the number of tasks increases, so does the level of contextual interference. This is the effect on the performer when an athlete must practice multiple tasks, simultaneously. The response to this is heavily dependent on the skill level of the performer with novice athletes typically struggling to cope with high levels of contextual interference (21). It may therefore be appropriate to use blocked practice when looking to train change-of-direction skills, particularly with novice athletes, and utilize more random practice with more advanced athletes looking to train perceptual-scanning skills. This should be an important consideration for practitioners when programming agility training (11).
Figure 1. Relationship between Contextual Interference and the Number of tasks.
Blocked Agility Training Literature
Table 1: Study Characteristics – Blocked Practice
These two studies both found greater improvements in closed change-of-direction tasks from blocked programming. It should be noted that both studies utilized novice participants. Bloomfield also used sport-specific training to provide the random dose of agility practice. It should be noted that both studies have limitations, as both used closed methods when testing their participants, failing to accommodate for the perceptual element of agility. Both studies highlight potential advantages for improving change-of-direction skills in novices using blocked practice.
Random Agility Training Literature
Table 2: Study Characteristics – Random Practice
Literature which has focused on random programming has identified the need for training to be specific for agility training to be effective. However, the level of technical proficiency required for this to be optimal is unclear. This provides a challenge for as it requires a subjective coaching judgement on the ability of the athlete. Porter and Magill (2010) suggested progressive methods be used with task difficulty matching the ability of the performer. Both random and blocked literature here is limited somewhat by the use of novice athletes. According to the research presented previously, sport-specific training may provide an adequate agility stimulus, but only in athletes who have technically mastered the necessary change-of-direction skills.
Skill-Level + Contextual Interference
Based on the information around random and blocked practice and contextual interference, we can start to form a model to inform practice:
Figure 2: Training Considerations for agility practice
This is a simple guide practitioners may choose to use when prescribing training based on the ability of their athletes.
Returning to the concept of contextual interference, Guadagnoll & Lee, (2004) came up with a model (Figure 3.) to try and identify the optimal challenge point for skill learning:
It suggests that there is a sweet spot of training relative to the practice performance and the potential learning benefit. As shown, these areas vary dependent on the skill level of the athlete – in a practical setting, this provides quite a challenge for a coach. As mentioned earlier, could a sport-specific session provide an adequate agility session? This would likely require collaboration between the skills-coach and S&C practitioner. This process may be aided by the use of GPS technology to add quantitative information to subjective observation. Practically speaking, it may be challenging to prescribe small-sided-games or similar as agility practice unless the entire squad of performers were technically proficient with their change-of-direction movement strategies.
Agility in TBS
Further to our central question, the model below can be used to prescribe practices with some different focuses. For example, our unopposed practice is probably where we are training change of direction skills in isolation, looking at mechanics and technique. But as we move along to an increasing number of players, the behavior and movement demands of the performers will have to change. Here we can increase contextual interference and start to see what constraints we may place on certain athletes.
Figure 4. Practice Design Model
The above model may be useful for practitioners looking to prescribe agility practice considering the ability of the athletes as well as the desired practice outcomes: technical/change-of-direction or perceptual/decision making.
Conclusion
In summary, both blocked and random programming have their advantages. As with many scenarios in S&C, things are contextual and where possible, training interventions should be kept as individualized and specific as possible. With this in-mind, using small-sided games as adequate agility training may provide a practical challenge, given the varied abilities of performers, desired outcomes from the skills coach and conditioning elements effecting repeated efforts of movement. The models provided in this review will hopefully provide practitioners with some takeaways to help prescribe practice, considering the athlete ability and the desired training outcomes – change-of-direction skill or perceptual scanning skill.
References:
BLOOMFIELD, J., POLMAN, R., OʼDONOGHUE, P., & McNAUGHTON, L. (2007). Effective Speed and Agility Conditioning Methodology for Random Intermittent Dynamic Type Sports. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 21(4), 1093–1100. https://doi.org/10.1519/00124278-200711000-00020
Charlesworth, B., Charlesworth, D., Barton, N. H., Egan-Shuttler, J. D., Edmonds, R., Eddy, C., O’Neill, V., Ives, S. J., Weston, M., Hibbs, A. E., Thompson, K. G., Spears, I. R., Carlsson, T., Wedholm, L., Nilsson, J., Carlsson, M., Aspenes, S., Kjendlie, P. L., Hoff, J., … Morouço, P. G. (2017). Effect of sequencing strength and endurance training in young male soccer players. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 10(2), 2447–2454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-017-3621-1
Clark, M. E., McEwan, K., & Christie, C. J. (2019). The effectiveness of constraints-led training on skill development in interceptive sports: A systematic review. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 14(2), 229–240. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747954118812461
Coffey, V. G., & Hawley, J. A. (2017). Concurrent exercise training: do opposites distract? Journal of Physiology, 595(9), 2883–2896. https://doi.org/10.1113/JP272270
Dawes, J. (2008). Creating open agility drills. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 30(5), 54–55. https://doi.org/10.1519/SSC.0b013e318189660a
Faigenbaum, A. D., Lloyd, R. S., MacDonald, J., & Myer, G. D. (2016). Citius, Altius, Fortius: Beneficial effects of resistance training for young athletes: Narrative review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 50(1), 3–7. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2015-094621
Graser, J. V., Bastiaenen, C. H. G., & van Hedel, H. J. A. (2019). The role of the practice order: A systematic review about contextual interference in children. PLoS ONE, 14(1), 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0209979
Guadagnoll, M. A., & Lee, T. D. (2004). Challenge Point: A Framework for Conceptualizing the Effects of Various Practice Conditions in Motor Learning. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36(2), 212–224. https://doi.org/10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224
Holmberg, P. M. (2015). Agility training for experienced athletes: A dynamical systems approach. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 37(3), 93–98. https://doi.org/10.1519/SSC.0000000000000145
Jayanthi, N. A., Labella, C. R., Fischer, D., Pasulka, J., & Dugas, L. R. (2015). Sports-specialized intensive training and the risk of injury in young athletes: A clinical case-control study. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 43(4), 794–801. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546514567298
Jeffreys, I. (2006). Motor learning - Applications for agility, part 2. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 28(6), 10–14. https://doi.org/10.1519/00126548-200612000-00001
Lloyd, R. S., & Oliver, J. L. (2015). The Youth Physical Development Model: A New Approach to Long-Term Athletic Development. Strength and Conditioning Journal, 34(3), 61–72.
Luke, A., Lazaro, R. M., Bergeron, M. F., Keyser, L., Benjamin, H., Brenner, J., D’Hemecourt, P., Grady, M., Philpott, J., & Smith, A. (2011). Sports-related injuries in youth athletes: Is overscheduling a risk factor? Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 21(4), 307–314. https://doi.org/10.1097/JSM.0b013e3182218f71
Mikkola, J., Rusko, H., Izquierdo, M., Gorostiaga, E. M., & Häkkinen, K. (2012). Neuromuscular and cardiovascular adaptations during concurrent strength and endurance training in untrained men. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 33(9), 702–710. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0031-1295475
Myer, G. D., Ford, K. R., Di Stasi, S. L., Barber Foss, K. D., Micheli, L. J., & Hewett, T. E. (2015). High knee abduction moments are common risk factors for patellofemoral pain (PFP) and anterior cruciate ligament (ACL) injury in girls: Is PFP itself a predictor for subsequent ACL injury? British Journal of Sports Medicine, 49(2), 118–122. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2013-092536
Porter, J. M., & Magill, R. A. (2010). Systematically increasing contextual interference is beneficial for learning sport skills. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28(12), 1277–1285. https://doi.org/10.1080/02640414.2010.502946
Renshaw, I., Davids, K., Shuttleworth, R., & Chow, J. Y. (2009). Insights from ecological psychology and dynamical systems. Theory can underpin a philosophy of coaching. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 40(4), 580–602.
Rössler, R., Donath, L., Verhagen, E., Junge, A., Schweizer, T., & Faude, O. (2014). Exercise-Based Injury Prevention in Child and Adolescent Sport: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Sports Medicine, 44(12), 1733–1748. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-014-0234-2
Stracciolini, A., Casciano, R., Levey Friedman, H., Stein, C. J., Meehan, W. P., & Micheli, L. J. (2014). Pediatric sports injuries: A comparison of males versus females. American Journal of Sports Medicine, 42(4), 965–972. https://doi.org/10.1177/0363546514522393
Wilson, J. M., Marin, P. J., Rhea, M. R., Wilson, S. M. C., Loenneke, J. P., & Anderson, J. C. (2012). Concurrent training: A meta-analysis examining interference of aerobic and resistance exercises. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 26(8), 2293–2307. https://doi.org/10.1519/JSC.0b013e31823a3e2d
Wulf, G., Chiviacowsky, S., Schiller, E., & Ávila, L. T. G. (2010). Frequent external-focus feedback enhances motor learning. Frontiers in Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00190
Comments